ALCOHOL AND THE PREFRONTAL CORTEX
Table Of Contents
Thesis Abstract
<p>
</p><div><p> <b>ABSTRACT</b><br></p><p>The prefrontal cortex occupies the anterior portion of the frontal lobes and is thought to be one of the most complex anatomical and functional structures of the mammalian brain. Its major role is to integrate and interpret inputs from cortical and sub-cortical structures and use this information to develop purposeful responses that reflect both present and future circumstances. This includes both action-oriented sequences involved in obtaining rewards and inhibition of behaviors that pose undue risk or harm to the individual. Given the central role in initiating and regulating these often complex cognitive and behavioral responses, it is no surprise that alcohol has profound effects on the function of the prefrontal cortex. In this chapter, we review the basic anatomy and physiology of the prefrontal cortex and discuss what is known about the actions of alcohol on the function of this brain region. This includes a review of both the human and animal literature including information on the electrophysiological and behavioral effects that follow acute and chronic exposure to alcohol. The chapter concludes with a discussion of unanswered questions and areas needing further investigation.</p></div><div><strong>Keywords </strong>Executive function, Orbitofrontal cortex, Working memory, Persistent Activity, Up-States</div>
<br><p></p>
Thesis Overview
<p><b>1.1 INTRODUCTION </b></p><p>
It is now widely accepted that alcohol and other addictive drugs act within the mesolimbic
dopamine (DA) system of the brain. This system originates in the ventral tegmental area
(VTA) and projects to various limbic structures, including the nucleus accumbens (NAc),
amygdala, and hippocampus. Most notably, it is thought that the positive reinforcing effects
of drugs of abuse relate to enhanced DA neurotransmission particularly within the NAc.
Therefore, virtually all models of the addiction neurocircuitry feature the mesolimbic DA
system as central to the addictive process. However, evidence gained over the past decade or
more suggests that drug-induced changes in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) also critically
regulate drug and alcohol addiction (Everitt and Robbins, 2005; Kalivas and Volkow, 2005;
Kalivas, 2009). This evidence comes from diverse studies that include human and animal
behavioral work, brain imaging, electrophysiology, and molecular and cellular observations.
Whereas a comprehensive review of the role of the PFC in addiction is beyond the scope of
this chapter, our aim is to provide a review of the literature regarding the effects of acute and
chronic alcohol exposure on PFC structure and function. Consistent with the accumulating
evidence implicating the PFC in addiction to opiates, cocaine, and other psychostimulants, it
is our contention that current models of the neurocircuitry of alcohol addiction should
prominently include ethanol-induced alterations in PFC structure and function. We further
suggest that long-lasting alterations in the executive function of the PFC and its associated
networks may play an equal or even greater role in alcohol addiction and relapse to drinking
than changes within the mesolimbic DA system.
<br></p><p>
<b>II. Anatomy of the Prefrontal Cortex </b></p><p>The cerebral cortex of humans and nonhuman primates is separated into a frontal and
posterior region by the Rolandic fissure. The primary motor cortex (area 4) is a relatively
narrow cortical area that lies immediately anterior to this fissure, and immediately anterior
to the primary motor cortex is the premotor cortex (area 6). The PFC makes up the
remaining anterior pole of the frontal cortex (Fig. 1, shaded area) and is divided into three
interconnected subregions known as the lateral PFC, the medial/cingulate PFC, and the
orbital PFC (commonly referred to as the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC)). Although overlapping
but not identical, the term ventromedial PFC is sometimes encountered in the literature
instead of OFC.
The PFC was originally termed the “frontal granular cortex” based upon the presence of a
cortical granular layer IV and a location rostral to the agranular premotor cortex. However,
subsequent comparisons across species of other cortical areas with known functional
homology soon revealed that defining cortical regions based upon a granular versus
agranular cytoarchitecture was invalid, and current definitions of the PFC utilize functional
homology and anatomical connectivity across species. With regard to connectivity, the
presence of dense reciprocal projections between the mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus
along with certain cortico-cortical connections is now considered the major defining
structural feature of the PFC that is applicable to all mammalian species (Fuster, 2008).
While it must be kept in mind that there are generally no neat boundaries separating the
subregions of the PFC, the human PFC can be grossly localized to specific Brodmann areas
of the frontal cortex. The lateral PFC is composed principally of parts or all of Brodmann
areas 8, 9, 10, and 46, and the medial PFC is composed principally of parts of areas 8, 9, 10,
and 12, with areas 24, 25, and 32 forming the anterior cingulate (ACC). The orbitofrontal
cortex (OFC) is the ventral portion of the frontal cortex on the dorsal surface of the orbit and
is comprised of areas 11, 12, 13, and 14.
The PFC is extensively interconnected not only between cortical layers but also between
subregions, and this connectivity helps to define distinct neural networks that exert
behavioral control. The portion of the lateral PFC dorsal to the fundus of the principal sulcus
receives afferent projections mainly from the medial PFC and belongs to the dorsolateral
PFC network, whereas that portion that lies ventral to the principal sulcus receives afferent
projections mostly from the OFC and belongs to the ventrolateral (sometimes referred to as
orbitoventral) PFC network (Barbas and Pandya, 1989; Ongur and Price, 2000). In addition
to reciprocal connections with the thalamus, the lateral PFC is connected either directly or
indirectly with virtually all areas of the neocortex and hippocampus and has dense efferent
projections to the dorsal caudate nucleus (e.g., dorsal striatum). There are distinct medial
and orbital PFC networks that are also characterized by cortico-cortical connections and
connections with brain regions outside the PFC. The medial PFC is connected with the
thalamus and sends efferents to the hypothalamus and periaqueductal gray, and belongs to a
network that plays a major role in autonomic and somatic responses to emotional stimuli.
The ACC receives similar reciprocal projection as the medial PFC but is thought to belong
to a network involved in attentional processing and conflict monitoring. The OFC is densely
connected with the basal ganglia, amygdala, and other prefrontal areas, and belongs to a
network that mediates motivational and affective aspects of behavior. The medial and OFC
networks are sometimes grouped together as the orbitomedial PFC (OMPFC). The medial
PFC and OFC are phylogenetically older regions, whereas the lateral PFC is a
phylogenetically newer structure and provide substrates for executive cognitive functions.
The role of the PFC in behavioral control is discussed in greater detail in the following
section.
<br></p><p>
The extent to which rats have a PFC has been the subject of debate (Uylings et al., 2003;
Seamans et al., 2008). The main issue of controversy is whether rats have a prefrontal area
that is comparable with the dorsolateral PFC of primates. In their review of the extant
human, nonhuman primate, and rodent literature, Uylings et al. (2003) concluded that,
similar to primates, rodents do in fact have a region of the frontal cortex that can be defined
both anatomically and functionally as PFC. They provide a strong argument that rats have a
functionally divided prefrontal cortex that includes not only features of the medial and
orbital areas in primates, but also some features of the primate dorsolateral PFC. In a more
recent review, Seamans et al. (2008) took this a step further by suggesting that the rat medial
PFC combines elements of the primate dorsolateral PFC and ACC at a rudimentary level
that in primates may have formed the building blocks required for abstract rule encoding
during evolutionary expansion of the PFC dorsally.
<br></p><p>
As in primates, the rodent PFC is located in the anterior pole of the frontal cortex and is
loosely defined as the ACC, the medial PFC, and the OFC, as well as portions of the
agranular insular cortex. The rodent medial PFC is subdivided into a dorsally located
prelimbic mPFC (PrL-mPFC) and ventrally located infralimbic mPFC (IfL-mPFC)
subregion. The OFC is also subdivided into the medial, ventral, lateral, and dorsolateral
orbitofrontal regions. The connectivity of the rodent PFC has common features with that of
primates, including a dorsal-to-ventral gradient of projection patterns. For example, the PrLmPFC sends dense projections to the dorsal striatum that transitions to the ventral stratum
(nucleus accumbens) as one moves ventrally through the IfL-mPFC to the OFC.
<br></p><p>
<b>III. The Executive Function of the Prefrontal Cortex </b></p><p>The perception–action cycle, originally described by Arbib in 1985, is a behavioral construct
of circular information flow that describes the interaction of an organism with its
environment allowing it to carry out the orderly sequencing of goal-directed actions (Arbib,
1985). As pointed out by Fuster (2008), an important element of this construct is that it
includes internal feedback from effectors to sensors to provide representations of current
actions to sensory structures to modulate further input. In anatomical terms, there are two
cognitive networks of this cycle (Fuster, 2008). One is the perceptual network of the
posterior cortex and the other is the executive network of the frontal cortex. Although these
networks are structurally distinct, they are not functionally independent and there is a great
deal of information exchange between them. Furthermore, the cortical components of these
networks do not act independent of subcortical structures. At the apex of the hierarchical
organization of the perception–action cycle is the PFC.
<br></p><p>
Before proceeding further with a discussion of executive function, we should note there are
different models of cognitive networks of the PFC. Prominent are variations of modular
concepts where cognitive representations are essentially ascribed to different neural
connections and anatomical locations (e.g., localization of working memory to the
dorsolateral PFC) (Norman and Shallice, 1986). In contrast is the model of large-scale
cortical networks first suggested by Bressler and expanded upon by Fuster (Bressler, 1995;
Fuster, 2008). In this model, while cognitive networks may have foci, to one degree or
another, in specific subregions of the PFC, they are widely distributed and bind together
neuronal assemblies from widespread regions of the cortex during information processing.
Importantly, a network can subserve different cognitive needs (e.g., memories), and
different neurons or groups of neurons can participate in, or belong to, different networks
depending upon the particular ongoing cognitive demands. The formation of these networks
is presumably dependent upon attractor dynamics of cell assemblies that encode specific
actions or memories.
<br></p><p>
The primary function of the PFC can be summarized as the temporal organization of
behavior. Executive function involves attention, planning, and decision making, and is how
the PFC organizes behavior in a sequential manner. This occurs in large part through the
dynamic interaction of two parallel networks of the PFC—an “executive” network with a
primary location in the dorsolateral PFC (or medial PFC in rodents) and a “limbic” network
primarily contained in the OFC (Fig. 2). These networks mediate higher-order behaviors
such as purposeful goal-directed actions, language, and reasoning. These networks are
required to perform a number of functions for appropriate control of goal-directed behavior
(outlined by Moghaddam and Homayoun, 2008). In summary, they must (1) detect
situations that demand mediation, (2) direct selective attention to stimuli relevant to this
situation, (3) suppress distractions caused by irrelevant stimuli, (4) bring on line relevant
past memories, (5) plan behavioral sequence based upon these memories and the present
relevant stimuli, and (6) encode the preparatory set that leads to motor execution of the
appropriate behavior. As noted above, the executive network is thought to be primarily
localized within the dorsolateral PFC, and as such this structure is considered to be the
ultimate regulator of goal-directed actions. However, the ACC also critically contributes to
many aspects of executive functioning by mediating attention to action and conflict
monitoring. Also as noted previously, the OFC is extensively connected with sensory
cortical areas and limbic structures, and it is thought that this network plays a role in
integrating and determining the salience of information about environmental contingencies.
It plays a particularly important role in inhibitory control of inappropriate behaviors by
relaying processed information to the executive control network of the dorsolateral PFC.
Thus, the executive and limbic networks function in concert to orchestrate behaviors
particularly related to context and expected outcomes (e.g., expectation of a goal-directed
action).
<br></p><p>
Any discussion of the executive function of the PFC would be remiss without at least
mentioning working memory, one of the major executive cognitive functions of the PFC.
Working memory is a form of sustained attention for the processing of prospective action.
Thus, working memory involves the maintenance and manipulation of task-relevant
information in the service of planning, problem solving, and predicting forthcoming events
(Unterrainer and Owen, 2006; D’Esposito, 2007). Working memory is therefore a form of
“active” memory involving sustained attention that is focused on an internal representation
and can be distinguished from short-term memory. Although short-term memory often
functions in the service of working memory (e.g., bringing a short-term memory online for
the purpose of planning and predicting forthcoming events), it is classically viewed as
temporary memory storage prior to its storage in long-term memory. Closely related to
working memory is attentional “set”, which is essentially attention to motor activity and is
used to plan a sequence of forthcoming actions. In a sense, working memory is the
representation of the near past whereas set is a representation of the near future (Fuster,
2008), and together they are critical for the temporal organization of behavior.</p><p>Glutamatergic neurotransmission is the primary regulator of activity-dependent synaptic
modifications that underlie experience-dependent brain plasticity (Chandler, 2003). As will
be discussed in the next section, both acute and chronic alcohol exposure significantly
impact glutamatergic neurotransmission in the PFC. In addition, results from several studies
suggest that plastic processes of associative learning overlap and interact with those that
underlie the development of addictive behaviors (Berke and Hyman, 2000). Addiction is
clearly a complex disease that involves motivational and higher-order cognitive processes
that initiate and control goal-directed behaviors, and accumulating evidence implicates
altered glutamatergic neurotransmission mediated by projections to and from the prefrontal
cortex in the neuroplasticity of addiction (Jentsch and Taylor, 1999; Kalivas, 2009). The
PFC is highly integrated into the addiction neurocircuitry. Addictive behaviors, such as
those associated with alcohol abuse and alcoholism, include loss of control over
consumption and relapse to drinking. The PFC normally exerts “top-down” (e.g.,
information derived from prior experience) inhibitory control over internal and external
sensory-driven compulsive behaviors. Increasing evidence suggests that continued drug
exposure leads to attenuation of the ability of the PFC to monitor and inhibit these
behaviors, with eventual loss of inhibitory control over drinking. Volkow and colleagues
have conceptualized this loss of inhibitory control by the PFC as a syndrome of “impaired
response inhibition and salience attribution” (Goldstein and Volkow, 2002; Volkow et al.,
2003). This syndrome is envisioned to encompass an integrated cluster of addictive
behaviors that depend upon interaction with the PFC. An important feature of the PFC is
that it is functionally and structurally adaptable, and a major component of cortical cognitive
processing is that it is highly influenced by “knowledge” of past experiences. Thus, reward
information such as that provided by the VTA dopaminergic system has a pervasive and
lasting influence on the activity of the PFC.
<br></p>